Rotterdam school of Management, Erasmus University compact logo

Article: Thursday, 1 August 2024

Nudges are used in lots of ways in the public sector; they are the application of behavioural science to influence – in a predictable way – the choices that people make. A nudge changes the presentation of choices so people are more likely to pick the option that benefits them. But their effectiveness varies according to their setting. Some of the reasons why nudges don’t work the same on everyone have been explained by Assistant Professor Antonia Krefeld-Schwalb from Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus University (RSM) and co-researchers Eli Rosen Sugerman and Prof. Eric J. Johnson of Columbia Business School in their paper Exposing omitted moderators: Explaining why effect sizes differ in the social sciencespublished in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America.

Policymakers increasingly rely on behavioural science to influence responses to global challenges such as climate change or health crises. But their interventions often exert very different effects according to who they’re used on and where they are encountered – and this has become an important problem. The use of behavioural science has been under Increasing scrutiny and criticism recently because of failed replications and high-level fraud cases in the field. Dr Krefeld-Schwalb’s large-scale study aimed to embrace and explain the differences in nudges’ effects caused by differences in people and their situations. 

 

Differences across settings and places 

The researchers used online data collection to study the responses of 11,000 people to an assortment of nudges that are used for many behavioural interventions, and they found substantial divergences across settings and examples.  
 
They applied techniques for modelling the divergences, and introduced a framework that measured the often-overlooked moderating factors that can affect the nudge either in intensity or direction. When they did this, their framework allowed them to measure the effects of what they called ‘fluid intelligence’ and ‘crystallized intelligence’ alongside attentiveness and experience for nudges and interventions that were presented as texts. They noted the different sizes of the effects and were able to identify which moderating factors were responsible for the variations. They were also able to measure and describe some people’s cognitive variables. 

We found very large differences in the strengths of the effects on people’s behaviour. Some of those differences can be explained by the cognitive variables we measured.

In short, they found: 

1. People are all very distinct

People are more different than behavioural scientists might think.

2. Interventions effect people very differently

A particular intervention can work strongly on some people, but weakly on others. Some interventions have the opposite effect on different individuals.   

“We found very large differences in the strengths of the effects on people’s behaviour. Some of those differences can be explained by the cognitive variables we measured. For example, we observed that more intelligent people are more strongly affected by framing a choice as a loss or win,” said Dr Krefeld-Schwalb. 

Predicting the variances 

The researchers were then able to give a theoretical and empirical framework for understanding and predicting how moderating factors produce variances in the size of the nudge’s effect.   

The researchers’ findings can enable governments and businesses to choose the right interventions for a particular segment of society. When the cognitive variables of a target audience are understood, then nudging strategies can be developed that promote a particular choice. In policy making, behavioural science is becoming increasingly used, for example to increase sustainable or health-promoting behaviour.   

And while there are clear business and society benefits to nudging, there are still considerations to be taken into account:  

  • Consider differences in nudging effects

    Policy makers must consider the differences in nudging’s effects when designing ethical, fair and efficient policies to work most strongly on those Individuals that need to change the most. 

  • Measure cognitive variables to generalize effects

    Researchers studying the efficiency of interventions must measure cognitive variables to be able to generalise the observed effects on the entire population. 

dr. (Antonia) Krefeld-Schwalb
Assistant Professor
Rotterdam School of Management (RSM)
Erasmus University Rotterdam
Photo
Antonia Krefeld-Schwalb
Pile of books with vibrant bookmarks protruding from various pages, symbolizing in-depth research.

Related articles

RSM Discovery

Want to elevate your business to the next level using the latest research? RSM Discovery is your online research platform. Read the latest insights from the best researchers in the field of business. You can also subscribe to the newsletter to receive a bimonthly highlight with the most popular articles.

Do you want to learn more about this subject?

Check out these RSM education programmes

Your contact for more information:

Danielle Baan

Science Communication and Media Officer

Portrait of Erika Harriford-McLaren

Erika Harriford-McLaren

Corporate Communications & PR Manager

Erasmus University campus in autumn, showcasing its iconic red trees, viewed from across the campus pool.